9 Training Models Answer: Can Expertise be Accelerated?
This article summarizes 9 of the famous models of training from research to explore if expertise can be accelerated.
Accelerated expertise has been talk-of-the-town in the academic circle lately. Now the business world has started seeing the need for the expertise of their employees to be accelerated. In general, building expertise takes a long time. In previous posts, I tried to review the research perspective on stages one travels during his or her journey towards expertise acquisition. See posts: Mastery Demystified: How Do the Skills of a Novice Develop into Mastery? Demystifying Stages in Novice to Expert Transition: 7 Models from Research and 7 Phases of Development of A Newbie To Become An Expert and Beyond. Further, I explored several promising models and approaches to building the expertise of people. See the post: 5 Training Guidelines for Skill Acquisition Towards Unconscious Competence. Several researchers established the possibility to produce professionals at a higher level of proficiency using some special training strategies. But that stays as a possibility to large extent. There is a lack of any comprehensive mechanism for accelerating proficiency either through training or otherwise which instructional designers, training strategists, or training experts could use off-the-shelf and apply in an organizational context.
As stated by Hoffman, Andrews & Feltovich (2012), “Empirical fact about expertise (i.e., that it takes a long time) sets the stage for an effort at demonstrating the acceleration of the achievement of proficiency.” (p. 9).
Some famous models do attempt to provide “some” insight into how training can be used to build a certain level of proficiency of employees and accelerate it. I thought I summarize various models from the angle of how expertise development and acceleration of expertise.
Bloom’s Mastery Learning Model (1968)
The educational theorist Carroll (1963) provided the first complete model of attaining proficiency in her “Mastery learning model”. She challenged traditional educational philosophy stating that ‘the learner will succeed in learning a given task to the extent that he spends the time that he needs to learn the task’ (p. 725). Carroll used certain factors like aptitude, or time needed to learn the task under ideal instruction, ability to understand instruction, perseverance, and external conditions like the time allowed for learning, and the quality of instruction. He speculated that the majority of learners will be successful in gaining mastery in learning by a suitable combination of these factors and systematically maximizing the time allowed for learning.
Boom further extended this theory and has been mostly known by his name. The experiment conducted by Blooms (1968) argued that with proper condition of learning and time given to learners almost all learners were able to demonstrate desired performance. Inherently this approach builds deliberate practice into its training philosophy. The fundamental premise is to continue to allow the trainee to practice until he has demonstrated desired standards of performance. Time is usually not the constraint in such a situation but the mastery of some skills to a certain level is the target.
The mastery learning principle has been used and demonstrated mostly in an educational setting by some researchers (Reezigt & Weide 1992; Anderson 1994; Motamedi & Sumrall 2000; Guskey 2001). Recently, several years after its original introduction, this model resurfaced again with the name ‘proficiency-based training’ in several surgical (Stefanidis, Korndorffer, Sierra, et al., 2005; Scott, Ritter, Tesfay, 2008; Brydges, Kurashashi, Brummer, et al, 2008) and military (Salas et al., 1998) research to develop proficient performers through training interventions. Pilot training has recently started using this methodology by lifting the restrictions on the same number of hours of practice for all trainees but actually tracking progress by task (Stewart and Dohme, 2005).
The outcome of proficiency-based training is that it is possible to achieve a constant level of mastery across several trainees and make it independent of time or the number of practice trials. During this learner is engaged in deliberate practice in which learners engage in repetitive performance, receive rigorous assessment, and receive informative feedback (Ericsson et al, 1993). Proficiency metrics for training tasks can provide the external motivation necessary to engage them in the skills acquisition process in ways that simply passing time or performing some arbitrary number of practice repetitions cannot. I will be writing about this model in one of my next posts.
The challenge with this approach is that it completely disregards any limit on the time needed to achieve desired proficiency level, thus faster time-to-proficiency is not even the purpose of believers in this philosophy.
Gardner’s Multiple Intelligence Model (1973, 1983)
Mostly known for the 6 intelligence people possess, the theory basically established that people have certain preferred learning styles, as well as their behavioral and working styles, and their natural strengths. The types of intelligence that a person possesses indicate not only a person’s capabilities but also the manner or method in which they prefer to learn and develop their strengths – and also to develop their weaknesses (quoted from Alan Chapman). Most fundamentally Gardner used this theory to define how education should be delivered considering people’s capabilities from 6 different angles.
Gardner’s theory actually established that training can have a dramatic effect on performance provided correct training methods are used to teach the concepts and skills pertaining to 6 different intelligence types. In summary, multiple intelligence can be created using appropriate training methods.
Dreyfus & Dreyfus’ Novice to Expert Skill Acquisition (1986)
The Dreyfus model is based on the notion that the acquisition of skill is a continuous process and skill is transformed by experience and mastery, and this then brings a change in performance. They propose that a learner will pass through five distinct levels of proficiency in skill acquisition: novice, advanced beginner, competence, proficiency, and expertise. As a novice progresses, he acquires more and more situational understanding and is able to exert his intuition in several situations. According to Dreyfus and Dreyfus (1986), the most important difference between the levels of expertise is the gradual shift from analysis to intuition and the grade of involvement. See the detailed review of this model in my previous post: 7 Phases of Development of A Newbie To Become An Expert and Beyond.
Through the concept of performance as a continuum, this model established that expertise is on the higher end of the spectrum. According to Dreyfus’s (1986) model, experts don’t apply rules or use any maxims or guidelines. He rather has an intuitive grasp of situations based on his deep tacit understanding. One key aspect of this level is that an individual relies on intuition and an analytical approach is used only in new situations or unrecognized problems not earlier experienced. Experience-based deep understanding provides him with a very fluid performance. At this stage, skills become automatic that even an expert is not aware of it. Based on prior experience, they can even come up with solutions for new, never-experienced-before situations (DiBello, Lehman, Missldine, 2011). “Experts” adopt a contextual approach to problem-solving and understand the relative, non-absolute nature of knowledge. This ability distinguishes the “expert” from the “proficient” practitioner (D’Youville College, n.d.). Reflection comes naturally and experts solve problems almost unconsciously.
The model, however, does not provide any insight into the mechanism or the training methods to help a novice to move from one stage to another and develop into an expert. There is also no indication of how the acquisition of expertise can be accelerated with training techniques.
Ericsson’s Expert Performance Model (1994)
In the Scientific study of exceptional or expert performance in various domains, Ericsson’s Expert Performance Model theorizes that deliberate practice is the fundamental mode of achieving expertise – and by expertise, he meant ‘world class’ expertise. Ericsson, Krampe, and Tesch-Romer (1993) explain that deliberate practice is highly individualized training on tasks selected by a qualified teacher for the purposes of building expertise in an individual. They further hypothesized that individual differences in expert performance could be attributed to the amount of deliberate practice.
Deliberate practice is not just practice or any other domain-related activity like work or on-the-job training event (Ericsson & Charness, 1994). Ericson (2006) distinguishes between routine practice and deliberate practice. “The core assumption of deliberate practice is that expert performance requires the opportunity to find suitable training tasks that the performer can master sequentially….typically monitored by a teacher or coach” (Ericsson, 2006, p.692). There are four components: focused goals which are determined by a teacher in order to improve a specific aspect of performance; concentration and effort; feedback from a teacher comparing actual to desired performance; and further opportunities for practice.
The theory further postulated that it takes 10,000 hours or 10 years (20 hours for 50 weeks a year for ten years = 10,000) of intense training and ‘deliberate practice’ to become an expert in almost anything.
The model mostly provides some scientific observations on how someone becomes an expert rather than how someone can be a “made” expert through training. In earlier attempts, Ericsson (1994) highlighted this gap by stating that: “Although these studies [on expertise] have revealed how beginners acquire complex cognitive structures and skills that circumvent the basic limits confronting them, researchers have not uncovered some simple strategies that would allow nonexperts to rapidly acquire expert performance.” (p.737).
The basic premise of this theory is that it takes longer to build expertise through correctly designed deliberate practice. Some later works on this model did indicate how intense training could build a deliberate practice to develop an individual into an expert. However, the journey is still believed to be very long with no shortcuts.
Sternberg’s Developing Expertise Model (1998)
Among several models for training design, the developing expertise model by Sternberg (1999) is quite useful to define training strategies. He presented a model of developing expertise that has five key elements: metacognitive skills, learning skills, thinking skills, knowledge, and motivation. Fundamentally his model is based on cycling and interactions of several skills together to achieve expertise Motivation is believed to drive metacognitive skills which in turn activate learning and thinking skills, which then provide feedback to the metacognitive skills, enabling one’s level of expertise to increase (Sternberg, 1985). The declarative and procedural knowledge acquired through thinking skills and learning skills also contributes toward the acquisition of expertise.
Talking about expertise, Sternberg (1998) disagrees that deliberate practice is the exclusive aspect of the acquisition of expertise. He adds that 5 skills drive the cycle time to expertise. Sternberg states (1999) that “the novice works toward expertise through deliberate practice. But this practice requires an interaction of all five of the key elements. At the center, driving the elements is motivation”.
I wrote about this model in my other post: 6 Practical Training Strategies from Sternberg’s Developing Expertise Model.
Though the model does not explicitly say it, an important postulation of this model is that all of these skills are modifiable and trainable. And there are some postulations that can be extended to accelerate expertise. Read my post on this topic.
Jacob’s S-OJT Model for Expertise Development (2002)
Jacob’s S-OJT model is believed to be an explicit and direct training approach to build expertise through on-the-job training of the employees as opposed to classroom training. S-OJT, short for structured on-the-job, is a structured process of defining, tracking, monitoring, and managing OJT to develop workplace expertise.
Typically, organizations arrange for on-the-job training for employees. On-the-job training is one of the ways to provide an employee with needed field experience and time to practice ensuring that employees have the competence to meet current and future work expectations. The employee learns several tasks during on-the-job training. He learns at a rate that is determined by a number of cases, issues, time to practice, and opportunities to further his skills (Jacobs & Bu-Rahmah, 2012). However, when OJT is not planned it usually happens on a “spur of the moment” basis leaving inexperienced employees to trial-and-error, imitation of others, or in some other way attempt to acquire the knowledge and skills to accomplish the task or do the job. This model was an attempt to streamline informal and class learning at the workplace.
“Specifically, structured OJT is a planned process of developing task-level expertise by having an experienced employee train a novice employee at or near the actual work setting” (Jacobs, 1999, p. 608). The s-OJT model fundamentally asserted the need to develop high levels of employee competence, or expertise, in the workplace. The model provided a six-step process used to design and implement S-OJT programs.
The important postulation of this model is that by correctly structuring the OJT, workforce expertise can be built and even can be accelerated.
I will be soon writing about the design of OJT and the implications of OJT in one of my next posts as well.
Alexander’s Acclimation to Proficiency Model (2003)
Alexander (2003) in her studies to develop proficiency in undergraduate students in an educational setting postulated that by using the correct synergy of three components namely: knowledge (domain and topic), strategic processing (surface and deep), and interest (long-term and situational) suitable training interventions can be designed to move learner to proficiency /expertise stage (as cited by Baker, 2006). The knowledge component is subdivided into domain knowledge (breadth of knowledge within a field) and topic knowledge (specific items or instances of knowledge within the scope of domain knowledge). The strategy processing component addresses the development of more sophisticated learning strategies from surface-level strategies as he/she progresses toward expertise. The third component interest has two forms of individual interest, which are long-term interest in a domain, and situational interest which is short-term and relates to the immediate situation.
She asserted that knowledge, strategic processing, and interest are the three components that interact with each other as the individual progresses toward expertise.
This study was an important study extending the postulation that it is possible to teach proficiency using correct training strategies. The main use of this model is managing students’ progress and providing them with a practical instructional environment.
Fadde’s Expertise-based Training Model (2009):
Fadde (2009) positioned Expertise based Training as an instructional design theory that draws upon theory, findings, and methods of expertise research to create methods to hasten the development of advanced learners into experts. The model integrated several concepts of training for complex learning and expertise such as part-task approach and representative tasks and drew upon novice-expert studies to develop Expertise Based Training (XBT) Model.
He states that two main principles of XBT are that instructional activities can be designed by repurposing expertise-novice tasks to systematically train key cognitive sub-skills of expertise and that targeted training of key cognitive skills can hasten learners along their individual paths to expertise. The key to success is to underline the key cognitive sub-skills which represent the expertise,
This study established that training strategies could be positioned to develop proficiency through training interventions. The application of the training model, as asserted by Fadde, is mostly in the intuitive expertise domain, more specifically intuitive decision making an aspect of expertise such as pattern recognition. This model has good training implications in that expertise can be trained.
William and Rosenbaum’s Learning Paths Model (2004)
Leaning paths, a proprietary approach developed by William and Rosenbaum in 2004 fundamentally asserts that by aligning the training, on-the-job assignments, and supporting infrastructure upon the hiring of an employee, organizations can reduce time-to-proficiency by 30% (Williams and Rosenbaum, 2004).
A learning path, by definition, is a chronological representation of an individual’s learning journey from Novice to Expert in a specific job role. Learning Paths outline the step-by-step learning process learners should follow to achieve the required level of job proficiency. They also include timing and completion checkpoints. They support the actions performers need to take to acquire and demonstrate specific job competencies. The learning Paths model guides them through what they need to do in order to complete the learning activities assigned in their learning path, when they need to complete each learning activity and in what recommended order, and how long it will take them to complete the learning activities (LearnwareDesign.com)
The model emphasizes the upfront analysis to understand where an individual is in his journey of learning in the company, use that information to design a sequence of pre-requisites and assignments and learning events, and seek associated support from stakeholders to make the learning path successful from novice to expert. By taking this approach, designers can figure out which skills they can influence in which mode and they can identify how to leverage informal learning opportunities for coaching and mentorship and also build interactions with other experienced employees.
This model, fundamentally grounded in training methods in the learning journey of an individual is said to be applied at several organizations with up to 30% improvement in time-to-proficiency. The research grounds of this commercial model are not known– therefore it has not picked up the attention of researchers yet. Nevertheless model does establish that it is possible to accelerate the proficiency of employees, not solely by training but by aligning all the activities, environment, and other variables in the journey of learning.
Contemporary Accelerated Expertise / Accelerated Proficiency Methods
In 2012, in their paper ‘Accelerated Learning’, Hoffman, Feltovich, and Andrew (2012) appealed to the research community that: “Our vision is that methods for accelerating the achievement of proficiency, and even extraordinary expertise, might be taken to new levels such that one can accelerate the achievement of proficiency across the journeyman-to-expert span post-hiring.” (p. 9).
The latest in the research and hot on the shelf is the book ‘Accelerated Expertise: Training for High Proficiency in Complex World (2014)’ by Hoffman, Ward, Feltovitch, DiBello, Fiore, and Andrews. The work has consolidated the practical implications of various research studies by leading researchers on expertise like Alexander, Benner, Dreyfus, Ericsson, Dibello, Hoffman, Feltovich, Fadde, Fiore, Klien, Chi, Clark, Sternberg, Mayer, etc. to name a few.
This book is basically a collection of several ways expertise could be accelerated through training. Though the work does not advocate a single specific model to accelerate the expertise, it establishes that there are many facets to accelerating expertise in a complex world and it may need a range of strategies including accelerated learning, team-based approaches, practice, transfer, retention strategies, etc. Most importantly it opens the door for training professionals to think about how training techniques and strategies can be developed and implemented to ‘Accelerate Expertise’.
I have explored several aspects of accelerating expertise and proficiency from the research point of view. While I argued the need for accelerating proficiency for the business world in Time To Proficiency Metrics: Why Should It Matter to Business?, lately I have explored several different aspects of the process of accelerating expertise/proficiency and the challenges associated with it. For example, three posts deal with training-based solutions or methods to accelerate expertise as described in 4 Cognitive Training Methods To Build and Accelerate Expertise, 6 Training Strategies to Accelerate Expertise from Sternberg’s Model and Accelerated Expertise with Mentoring and Tough Cases. In recent times, several breakthrough models like 70:20:10 have evolved with the potential to accelerate the proficiency of employees. See post 9 Guidelines To Accelerate Time To Competence with 70:20:10 Framework.
SUGGESTED CITATION
Attri, RK (2017), ‘Exploring 9 Training Models That Answer The Question: Can Expertise be Accelerated?’, [Blog post], Speed To Proficiency Research: S2PRo©, Available online at <https://get-there-faster.com/blog/can-expertise-be-accelerated/>.
REFERENCES
- Alexander, P. A. (2003). The Development of Expertise: The Journey From Acclimation to Proficiency. Educational Researcher, 32(8), 10–14. doi:10.3102/0013189X032008010
- Anderson, S. A. (1994). Synthesis of research on mastery learning. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED 382 567).
- Baker, R. (2006). The Development of Expertise: The Journey from Acclimation to Proficiency, a critical review, Journal of Comprehensive Research, 4, 48
- Bloom, B. S. (1968). Learning for mastery. Evaluation Comment, 1(2), 1–12.
- Bloom, B. S. (1971a). Mastery learning. In J. H. Block (Ed.), Mastery learning: Theory and practice (pp. 47–63). Holt, NY: Rinehart & Winston.
- Bloom, B. S. (1974). An introduction to mastery learning theory. In J. H. Block (Ed.), Schools, society and mastery learning (pp. 3–14). New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.
- Bloom, B. S. (1985). Generalizations about talent development. In B. S. Bloom (Ed.), Developing talent in young people (pp. 507-549). New York: Ballantine Books.
- Brydges, R., Kurahashi, A., Brümmer, V., et al. (2008) Developing criteria for proficiency-based training of surgical technical skills using simulation: changes in performances as a function of training year. Journal of Americal Coll Surgical, 206, 205–211.
- Caroll, J.A. (1963). A model of School Learning. Teachers College Record. 64, 723-733
- Dreyfus, H. (2007). Design conference on the learning environment: keynote address from novice to expert. ACGME Bull April 2007: 6-8.
- Dreyfus, H. L. & Dreyfus, S. E. (2008). Beyond Expertise: some preliminary thoughts on mastery. In Klaus Nielsen, K. et al. (eds) A Qualitative Stance: Essays in honor of Steiner Kvale. pp 113-124. Aarhus University Press . Available at: http://www.ieor.berkeley.edu/People/Faculty/dreyfus-pubs/mastery.doc
- Dreyfus, H. L. (2004). A Phenomenology of Skill Acquisition as the basis for a Merleau-Pontian Nonrepresentationalist Cognitive Science. Berkeley, CA: University of California, Department of Philosophy. Online version: http://www.irafs.org/irafs_1/cd_irafs02/texts/dreyfus.pdf
- Dreyfus, H. L. and Dreyfus, S. E. (1986). Why skills cannot be represented by Rules. In. Sharley, Elis, Hardwood, and Chichester, Advances in cognitive science. pp.315-335.
- Dreyfus, H.L., (2008). On the Internet. 2nd edn. London: Routledge.
- Dreyfus, H.L., Dreyfus, S.E., (1986a). Mind over machine: The power of human intuition and expertise in the era of the computer. New York, NY: The Free Press.
- D’Youville College (n.d.) Stages of Professional Mastery, Piantanida, M., https://www.dyc.edu/academics/pharmacy/assessment/stages.aspx
- Ericsson, K. A. & Charness, N. (1994). Expert performance – Its structure and acquisition. American Psychologist 49, 8, 725-747.
- Ericsson, K. A. (2001). Attaining Excellence Through Deliberate Practice: insights from the study of expert performance, In Ferrari, M. (ed.) The Pursuit of Excellence Through Education, Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
- Ericsson, K. A. (2006). The influence of experience and deliberate practice on the development of superior expert performance. In Ericsson, Charness, Feltovich & Hoffman (eds.). The Cambridge Handbook of Expertise and Expert Performance (pp.683-703) New York: Cambridge University Press.
- Ericsson, K. A., Krampe, R. Th., & Heizmann, S. (1993). Can we create gifted people? In CIBA Foundation Symposium 178, The origins and development of high ability (pp. 222-249). Chichester, England: Wiley.
- Ericsson, K. A., Prietula, M. J., Cokely, E. T., (Jul-Aug 2007). The Making of an Expert The Making of an Expert. Harvard business review.
- Ericsson, K.A., Krampe, R.T., Tesch-Romer, C. (1993). The role of deliberate practice in the acquisition of expert performance. Psychology Review, 100, 363– 406.
- Fadde, P. J. (2009). Instructional design for advanced learners: Training expert recognition skills to hasten expertise. Educational Technology Research and Development, 57(3), 359–376. DOI (online first, 2007) 10.1007/s11423-007-9046-5.
- Fadde, P.J. (2009a). Expertise-Based Training: Getting More Learners Over the Bar in Less Time. Technology, Instruction, Cognition and Learning, 7, 171–197.
- Gardner, H. (1973). The arts and human development. New York: Wiley.
- Gardner, H. (1983). Frames of mind: The theory of multiple intelligences. New York: Basic Books.
- Guskey, T. R. (2001). Mastery learning. In N. J. Smelser & P. B. Baltes (Eds.), International Encyclopedia of Social and Behavioral Sciences (pp. 9372–9377). Oxford, England: Elsevier Science.
- Hoffman, R. R., Andrews, D. H., & Feltovich, P. J. (2012). What is “ Accelerated Learning ”? Cognitive Technology, 17 (1)
- Hoffman, R.R. (Ed.) (2007). Expertise Out of Context: Proceedings of the Sixth International Conference on Naturalistic Decision Making. Boca Raton FL: Taylor and Francis.
- Hoffman, R.R., Ward, P., Feltovich, P.J., DiBello, L., Fiore, S.M., Andrews, D.H., (2013) Accelerated Expertise: Training for High Proficiency in a Complex World, Psychology Press New York
- Jacobs, R. (1994). Case studies that compare the training efficiency and product quality of unstructured and structured OJT. In Phillips, J. (Ed.), The Return on Investment in Human Resource Development: Cases on the Economic Benefits of HRD.pp. 123-32. Alexandria, VA: American Society for Training and Development (ASTD).
- Jacobs, R. (2001), ‘‘Managing employee competence and human intelligence in global organizations’’, in Richter, F. (Ed.), Maximizing Human Intelligence in Asia Business: The Sixth Generation Project, Prentice-Hall, New York, NY.
- Jacobs, R. (2002a). Implementing structured on-the-job learning. In Jacobs, R. (Ed.), Implementing Structured On-the-job Learning: A Casebook, Alexandria, VA: American Society for Training and Development (ASTD).
- Jacobs, R. L., & Bu-Rahmah, M.J., (2012). Developing employee expertise through structured on-the-job training (S-OJT): an introduction to this training approach and the KNPC experience. Industrial and Commercial Training, 44(2), 75-84. DOI: 10.1108/00197851211202902
- Jacobs, R.L. (2003), Structured on-the-job Training: Unleashing Employee Expertise in the Workplace, San Francisco, CA: Berrett-Koehler
- Learning Paths (2013) How to Get Employees Up-to-Speed in Record Time. Learning Path International Canada: Tesolin, A., Available at: http://www.hr.com/en?t=/network/event/attachment.supply&s=17eX3NGoqKsYQOXQ&fileID=135766775926
- Motamedi, B., & Sumrall, W. J. (2000). Mastery learning and contemporary issues in education. Action in Teacher Education, 22(1), 22–32.
Image credits: Robust Pro.com